[visionlist] Forced Choice – please educate me

Mathias Hegele mathias.hegele at sport.uni-giessen.de
Tue Jan 21 10:28:30 -05 2025


Dear Dr. Bach,

since some of my colleagues (not in psychology, but rather in the 
humanities) frequently express their suspicion that younger colleagues 
use AI to arrive at outlandish views, I took the liberty of submitting 
your question/statement to ChatGPT, just out of curiosity. Below you can 
read what came out of it. Apparently, ChatGPT or AI in general does not 
seem to be responsible for the change in the understanding of nAFC tasks 
you describe :) (not that you claimed that...)

Best wishes from Giessen
Mathias Hegele

PS: It is particularly funny that it sort of offers to co-author a paper 
on this issue ;)


ChatGPT’s response:

You’ve raised an important and nuanced concern about the understanding 
and application of forced-choice paradigms in psychophysics. Let me 
break down and address the key points in your query:

1. The Historical Understanding of nAFC and "Equivalent" Alternatives

You are correct that traditionally, the forced-choice 
paradigm—especially n-alternative forced choice (nAFC)—was designed 
to mitigate bias or criterion effects by presenting equivalent 
alternatives. Equivalence ensures that the observer is not predisposed 
to favor one alternative due to inherent differences in their salience 
or likelihood. For instance:
In a 2AFC task (e.g., left vs. right or first vs. second interval), the 
alternatives are typically symmetric and balanced.
In more complex tasks (e.g., a Landolt C gap among multiple 
orientations), equivalence can be harder to achieve due to perceptual 
effects like the oblique effect.
Historically, equivalence was indeed seen as a critical feature of nAFC 
paradigms, as it justified the assumption that bias would not skew the 
results. This allowed researchers to directly infer sensitivity from 
performance without needing to disentangle bias and sensitivity 
measures.

2. Shift in Understanding or Application

Your observation that younger scientists might not understand nAFC 
paradigms in this way reflects a shift in either how the paradigm is 
taught or how its principles are applied. It’s possible that the 
emphasis on equivalence has waned, leading to looser interpretations of 
what constitutes "forced choice."
Tasks such as "seen/not seen" or "go/no-go" are, as you noted, 
fundamentally different because the alternatives are not equivalent:
"Seen/not seen" inherently invites a bias due to differing subjective 
criteria for detection.
"Go/no-go" relies heavily on the observer's decision threshold and is 
particularly sensitive to criterion effects.
These designs require explicit measures like d′ (sensitivity index) or 
other signal detection metrics to separate sensitivity from bias—a 
step that might not be consistently appreciated in modern 
interpretations.

3. Scientific Folklore and the Myth of Bias Elimination

The "scientific folklore" that forced-choice paradigms eliminate bias is 
a misinterpretation that can lead to flawed study designs and incorrect 
conclusions:
When alternatives are equivalent (e.g., standard nAFC): Bias effects are 
minimized because the task structure does not favor one response over 
another.
When alternatives are not equivalent (e.g., seen/not seen): Bias 
persists and must be accounted for explicitly, regardless of the "forced 
choice" label.
The assumption that merely adding a “not seen” button or another 
unequal option creates a forced-choice design is problematic. While 
technically such tasks might involve a forced choice, they fail to 
eliminate bias in the way that equivalent-alternative designs do.

4. The Real Problem: Perceptual Learning and Longitudinal Studies

Your concern about perceptual learning and bias in longitudinal studies 
is particularly salient. Perceptual learning often changes both 
sensitivity and decision criteria, which can confound results in poorly 
designed forced-choice tasks. If bias effects are not properly 
addressed, one might mistakenly attribute changes in performance to 
sensitivity improvements when they are actually due to shifts in 
criterion.
To avoid this pitfall:

Ensure equivalence in alternatives for nAFC tasks whenever possible.
If equivalence is impractical, incorporate signal detection measures 
(e.g., d′, criterion) or other analytic tools to separate sensitivity 
and bias.
Use complementary tasks or control conditions to validate that observed 
effects reflect changes in perceptual sensitivity rather than decision 
biases.

5. Practical Recommendations

- Teaching and Communication: It’s important to emphasize in training 
and publications that the true strength of nAFC paradigms lies in their 
equivalence of alternatives, which minimizes bias.
- Standardization of Terminology: Clarify the distinction between 
forced-choice paradigms with equivalent alternatives (true nAFC) and 
other designs (e.g., yes/no tasks with a “forced-choice” label) to 
prevent misuse or misunderstanding.
- Experimental Design: For studies where equivalence is challenging, be 
explicit about how bias will be addressed analytically (e.g., using 
signal detection theory).
- Awareness of Limitations: Encourage researchers to acknowledge the 
limitations of their forced-choice designs when equivalence is not 
achieved.

6. Your Intuition is Right

You are absolutely correct that forced-choice paradigms do not 
inherently eliminate bias unless the alternatives are equivalent. 
Without this equivalence, the assumption that performance solely 
reflects sensitivity breaks down, requiring additional analytical steps.

It may be valuable to publish or share these insights more widely, as 
they address a gap in understanding that has practical implications for 
the field. I’d be happy to assist in drafting a more formal exposition 
or any other next steps you envision!

On 21 Jan 2025, at 15:17, Michael Bach wrote:

> Dear Colleagues:
>
> I have the impression that the general understanding of a 
> psychophysical forced-choice paradigm has changed over the decades.
>
> To my understanding, bias/criterion effects can only be effectively 
> mitigated in an nAFC (n-alternative forced choice) design where the 
> alternatives are _equivalent_. Examples: the pattern is on the 
> right/left (balanced, of course), the stimulus was in the first/second 
> interval, or the gap of a Landolt C is in one of 8 directions. 
> “Equivalence” may be challenging to achieve, particularly in the 
> latter case, where the oblique effect may interfere.  Alternatives 
> such as “seen/not seen” or “go/no” are not equivalent and 
> necessitate measures (d’, …) to address bias/criterion.
>
> For me, nAFC implicitly implied equivalent alternatives, but was this 
> ever the case? Whatever, young scientists do not seem to understand it 
> this way.
>
> While this might be just seen as a nomenclature trifle, the real 
> problem arises due to the prevalent “scientific folklore” that a 
> forced-choice paradigm eliminates the effects of bias / criterion 
> change (which, in particular, occur through perceptual learning in 
> longitudinal studies). However, if forced-choice does not employ 
> _equivalent_ alternatives, this assertion is invalid.
>
> I have encountered opinions suggesting that the addition of a “not 
> seen” button constitutes forced choice (yes…, but:) and also 
> eliminates bias…
>
>
> Looking forward to your advice, best, Michael
> -- 
> https://michaelbach.de
>
>
> _______________________________________________
> visionlist mailing list
> visionlist at visionscience.com
> http://visionscience.com/mailman/listinfo/visionlist_visionscience.com
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://visionscience.com/pipermail/visionlist_visionscience.com/attachments/20250121/430997c0/attachment.html>


More information about the visionlist mailing list