[visionlist] Forced Choice – please educate me
Mathias Hegele
mathias.hegele at sport.uni-giessen.de
Tue Jan 21 10:28:30 -05 2025
Dear Dr. Bach,
since some of my colleagues (not in psychology, but rather in the
humanities) frequently express their suspicion that younger colleagues
use AI to arrive at outlandish views, I took the liberty of submitting
your question/statement to ChatGPT, just out of curiosity. Below you can
read what came out of it. Apparently, ChatGPT or AI in general does not
seem to be responsible for the change in the understanding of nAFC tasks
you describe :) (not that you claimed that...)
Best wishes from Giessen
Mathias Hegele
PS: It is particularly funny that it sort of offers to co-author a paper
on this issue ;)
ChatGPT’s response:
You’ve raised an important and nuanced concern about the understanding
and application of forced-choice paradigms in psychophysics. Let me
break down and address the key points in your query:
1. The Historical Understanding of nAFC and "Equivalent" Alternatives
You are correct that traditionally, the forced-choice
paradigm—especially n-alternative forced choice (nAFC)—was designed
to mitigate bias or criterion effects by presenting equivalent
alternatives. Equivalence ensures that the observer is not predisposed
to favor one alternative due to inherent differences in their salience
or likelihood. For instance:
In a 2AFC task (e.g., left vs. right or first vs. second interval), the
alternatives are typically symmetric and balanced.
In more complex tasks (e.g., a Landolt C gap among multiple
orientations), equivalence can be harder to achieve due to perceptual
effects like the oblique effect.
Historically, equivalence was indeed seen as a critical feature of nAFC
paradigms, as it justified the assumption that bias would not skew the
results. This allowed researchers to directly infer sensitivity from
performance without needing to disentangle bias and sensitivity
measures.
2. Shift in Understanding or Application
Your observation that younger scientists might not understand nAFC
paradigms in this way reflects a shift in either how the paradigm is
taught or how its principles are applied. It’s possible that the
emphasis on equivalence has waned, leading to looser interpretations of
what constitutes "forced choice."
Tasks such as "seen/not seen" or "go/no-go" are, as you noted,
fundamentally different because the alternatives are not equivalent:
"Seen/not seen" inherently invites a bias due to differing subjective
criteria for detection.
"Go/no-go" relies heavily on the observer's decision threshold and is
particularly sensitive to criterion effects.
These designs require explicit measures like d′ (sensitivity index) or
other signal detection metrics to separate sensitivity from bias—a
step that might not be consistently appreciated in modern
interpretations.
3. Scientific Folklore and the Myth of Bias Elimination
The "scientific folklore" that forced-choice paradigms eliminate bias is
a misinterpretation that can lead to flawed study designs and incorrect
conclusions:
When alternatives are equivalent (e.g., standard nAFC): Bias effects are
minimized because the task structure does not favor one response over
another.
When alternatives are not equivalent (e.g., seen/not seen): Bias
persists and must be accounted for explicitly, regardless of the "forced
choice" label.
The assumption that merely adding a “not seen” button or another
unequal option creates a forced-choice design is problematic. While
technically such tasks might involve a forced choice, they fail to
eliminate bias in the way that equivalent-alternative designs do.
4. The Real Problem: Perceptual Learning and Longitudinal Studies
Your concern about perceptual learning and bias in longitudinal studies
is particularly salient. Perceptual learning often changes both
sensitivity and decision criteria, which can confound results in poorly
designed forced-choice tasks. If bias effects are not properly
addressed, one might mistakenly attribute changes in performance to
sensitivity improvements when they are actually due to shifts in
criterion.
To avoid this pitfall:
Ensure equivalence in alternatives for nAFC tasks whenever possible.
If equivalence is impractical, incorporate signal detection measures
(e.g., d′, criterion) or other analytic tools to separate sensitivity
and bias.
Use complementary tasks or control conditions to validate that observed
effects reflect changes in perceptual sensitivity rather than decision
biases.
5. Practical Recommendations
- Teaching and Communication: It’s important to emphasize in training
and publications that the true strength of nAFC paradigms lies in their
equivalence of alternatives, which minimizes bias.
- Standardization of Terminology: Clarify the distinction between
forced-choice paradigms with equivalent alternatives (true nAFC) and
other designs (e.g., yes/no tasks with a “forced-choice” label) to
prevent misuse or misunderstanding.
- Experimental Design: For studies where equivalence is challenging, be
explicit about how bias will be addressed analytically (e.g., using
signal detection theory).
- Awareness of Limitations: Encourage researchers to acknowledge the
limitations of their forced-choice designs when equivalence is not
achieved.
6. Your Intuition is Right
You are absolutely correct that forced-choice paradigms do not
inherently eliminate bias unless the alternatives are equivalent.
Without this equivalence, the assumption that performance solely
reflects sensitivity breaks down, requiring additional analytical steps.
It may be valuable to publish or share these insights more widely, as
they address a gap in understanding that has practical implications for
the field. I’d be happy to assist in drafting a more formal exposition
or any other next steps you envision!
On 21 Jan 2025, at 15:17, Michael Bach wrote:
> Dear Colleagues:
>
> I have the impression that the general understanding of a
> psychophysical forced-choice paradigm has changed over the decades.
>
> To my understanding, bias/criterion effects can only be effectively
> mitigated in an nAFC (n-alternative forced choice) design where the
> alternatives are _equivalent_. Examples: the pattern is on the
> right/left (balanced, of course), the stimulus was in the first/second
> interval, or the gap of a Landolt C is in one of 8 directions.
> “Equivalence” may be challenging to achieve, particularly in the
> latter case, where the oblique effect may interfere. Alternatives
> such as “seen/not seen” or “go/no” are not equivalent and
> necessitate measures (d’, …) to address bias/criterion.
>
> For me, nAFC implicitly implied equivalent alternatives, but was this
> ever the case? Whatever, young scientists do not seem to understand it
> this way.
>
> While this might be just seen as a nomenclature trifle, the real
> problem arises due to the prevalent “scientific folklore” that a
> forced-choice paradigm eliminates the effects of bias / criterion
> change (which, in particular, occur through perceptual learning in
> longitudinal studies). However, if forced-choice does not employ
> _equivalent_ alternatives, this assertion is invalid.
>
> I have encountered opinions suggesting that the addition of a “not
> seen” button constitutes forced choice (yes…, but:) and also
> eliminates bias…
>
>
> Looking forward to your advice, best, Michael
> --
> https://michaelbach.de
>
>
> _______________________________________________
> visionlist mailing list
> visionlist at visionscience.com
> http://visionscience.com/mailman/listinfo/visionlist_visionscience.com
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://visionscience.com/pipermail/visionlist_visionscience.com/attachments/20250121/430997c0/attachment.html>
More information about the visionlist
mailing list